CHILD DEVELOPMENT |
Time: TT, 11:30 AM* Place: 58L (Studio 3) or off campus site *TV: WF, 3:40 A.M., KUHT Teaching Assistant: Jennifer Berglund 239A, Heyne E-mail: JBerg@Bayou.uh.edu Office hours: To be announced |
Office: 231D, Heyne: 713-743-8573 GGratch@uh.edu Office Hours: M, 2-4;Tu, 1-4
Mail: Department of Psychology
|
---|
Required for
purchase
Berger, K.S. & Thompson, R.A. (1995). The
Developing Person Through Childhood Adolescence
(4th ed.) New York: Worth.
Straub, R.A. & Brown, J. W. (1995). Study
Guide to Accompany Berger/Thompson. New York: Worth
Rubin, L.B. (1996), The Transcendent Child. New York: Harper Perennial.
This course is an introduction to the study of child development. It is intended to help the student understand the psychological development of children and the student’s own development. The course covers the period of the lifespan between conception and the onset of adolescence. Descriptions of how children develop and factors that cause or condition development are presented. Those descriptions are presented as contingencies rather than certainties. Development is a historical process, many-faceted and contingent. The facts we gather and the interpretations we make depend upon our time and theories. Not only do we have many good theories but they are disparate and they keep being revised. My hope is that you will welcome a journey into the worlds of childhood and those who study them. Perhaps the following quotation form G. Polya, a notable mathematician and teacher of mathematics, will convey the spirit in which we, you and I, can approach this course:
Textbook and Study Guide: All students are expected to read the assignments in the required text. If you keep up with the reading, then the lectures, demonstrations, films, and exams will make more sense to you, and you will be able to take an active roll in class. Before each lecture at least skim the appropriate chapter and read the review section of the chapter. The study guide will help you focus and organize your textbook reading.
Transcendent Child: This book presents case studies of people who grew up under circumstances that should have led them to be severely maladjusted and yet they somehow "transcended" that fate. You will read this book and write a report on it (see attached description).
My role in class will be to provide perspectives and contexts for your readings and to build on the text rather than review it. I will more than welcome questions about the required readings and will begin ach class by asking you whether there are specific aspects of the readings that you want clarified or discussed. Hopefully, you will come to each class prepared to identify the specific sections in the readings that you want considered. To help you follow my lectures, I will place out lines of the lectures on library reserve (3rd floor of UH library or Off-Campus site). You can check them out and copy them. Having a copy of the outlines in advance of my lectures should simplify note-taking and hopefully facilitate understanding.
CLASS SCHEDULE (dates of topics approximate; readings refer to textbook)
Live Class |
Broadcast Class |
Topic |
---|---|---|
8/26 | 8/29 | Introduction: Continuing controversies about
development
Readings: Chapter 1 |
8/28 | 9/3 | Discussion of Child Observation Project
Readings: Child observation handout |
9/2 | 9/5 | Knowing: The importance of
empirical and analytic methods
Readings: Chapter 1 |
9/4 | 9/10 | Knowing: The importance of theory—presuppositions,
history, synopsis of theories of child development
Note: Chapter 1 and 2 bear upon the subsequent readings, will be included in Test 2, and should be consulted throughout. |
9/9 | 9/12 | Knowing: Synopsis of theories of child
development continued.
Readings: Chapter 2 |
9/11 | 9/17 | Nature-Nurture: Heredity from historical
and Darwinian perspectives
Readings: Chapter 1 and 3 pps 77-91) |
9/16 | 9/19 | Nature-Nurture: Pre-natal development
from a Darwinian perspective
Readings: Chapter 1 and 4 (pps 109-123) |
9/18 | 9/24 |
Infancy-1: A nativist view reflexes, maturation,
and motor development
Readings: Chapter 6 |
9/23 | 9/26 | Infancy-1: An empiricist view—the case of vision
Readings: Chapter 7 |
9/25 | 10/1 |
Infancy-2: A "constructivist" view Piaget and the
development of sensorimore intelligence
Readings: Chapter 2 and 7 |
9/30 | 10/3 | Infancy-2: Piaget and the development of
conceptions of the world and self
Readings: Chapter 7 |
10/2 | 10/8 |
Infancy-3: Socioemotional development:
what do babies need: Want? Special importance of early experience? Psychoanalytic theory; Harlow’s monkey studies.
Readings: Chapter 1 and 8 (pps. 252-265) |
10/4 | 10/4 | ALL students are invited to meet at 9:00 A.M., Kiva, Farish Hall, UH to view a video program on infant emotions and to discuss the child observation project and other matters of interest |
10/7 | 10/10 | Infancy-3: Emotional milestones; review for Test 1
Readings: Chapter 8 (pps. 252-265) |
10/9 | 10/11* | Test 1
ITV: Test will held in Kiva, Farish Hall, UH at 9:00 A.M. *10/15 program will be a re-showing of program shown on 10/10 |
10/14 | 10/17 | Infancy-3: Ethology, imprinting, Bowlby’s
attachment theory, and measuring attachment
Readings: Chapter 1 and 8 |
10/16 | 10/22 |
Infancy-4: Contraversies about attachment "mother
stay home"; temperament, etc.
Readings: Chapter 8 and 9 |
10/21 | 10/24 |
Infancy-4: Parental conflicts of interest and child
abuse
Readings: Chapter 8 and 9 CHILD OBSERVATION REPORT DUE OCTOBER 21 |
10/23 | 10/29 | Language: What is a language? How does it develop?
Readings: pps 237-246 (ch. 7); pps 354-362 (ch.10); pps. 447-454 (ch. 13) |
10/28 | 10/31 |
Language and thought: Piaget and Vygotsky
the context their views of how intelligence
develops in childhood
Readings: Chapter 2 and 10 |
10/30 | 11/5 | Piaget’s studies of and theorizing about the
development of intelligence
Readings: Chapter 10 and 13 |
11/4 | 11/7 | Vygotshy’s reaction to Piaget: "Egocentric speech"
and the internalization of the tool of language and
its bearing on culture and schooling
Readings: Chapter 10 and 13 |
11/6 | 11/12 | Intelligence as IQ: Individual differences
in ability and failure and success in school
Readings: Chapter 1, 13, and 12 |
11/11 | 11/14 |
Intelligence as IQ: Some alternative ways to
think about individual differences in ability
"unnaturalness" of literacy and "exceptional effort"
Reading: Chapter 13 and 12 |
11/13 | 11/19 | Socioemotional development: The case of gender
socialization
Readings: Chapter 2 and 11 |
11/18 | 11/21 | Influential theories of gender socialization
Readings: Chapter 2 and 11 Transcendent Child Report due November 18 |
11/20 | 11/26 |
Parenting: Baumrind’s typology of "good" and "bad"
parenting; discipline and punishment
Readings: Chapter 1, 11, and 14 |
11/25 | 11/28 |
Parenting: Placing baumrind’s typology in the
context of social and economic changes
Readings: Chapter 1, 11, and 14 |
12/2 | 12/5 | "Peer pressure": the "culture of childhood"
and moral devleopment
Readings: Chapter 1, 11, and 14 |
12/4 | 12/10* |
Moral development continued; coda:
childhood resilience; review for Test 2
Readings: Chapter 14 *Program show on 12/10 will be a reshowing of the 12/5 program |
12/6 |
ITV students meet 12/6 at 9:00 A.M. Kiva, Farish
Hall for this class
NOTE: ITV students are welcome to attend the 12/4 studio class if 12/6 is difficult *Program show on 12/10 will be a reshowing of the 12/5 program |
|
12/18* | 12/18 | Test 2
Live class students: 12/18 at 11:00 A.M. ITV students: 12/18 at 11:00 A.M. in room 237 PGH, UH |
*Program show on 12/10 will be a reshowing of the 12/5 program |
It’s one thing to read about the facts and theories psychologists generate about children’s minds and quite another thing to participate in the process. The observations provide you with a opportunity to bridge the gap. You will administer 4 tasks to each of 2 children. Their ages will be about 4 and 5; no younger than 4 and no older than just turning 6. The different ages will give you a chance to see how children change in their willingness, coprehensio, and ability with regard to the tasks. (you can use 2 children of about the same age if you like; if so, close to age 5 is preferable.) There are two general ways to think about the children’s performances. One is in terms of how successfully they are relative to adult standards: Do they get "smarter" as they get older? Another is to focus on how they go about the tasks in their terms and what can we learn about how their minds work from the mistakes they make? The virtue of the latter approach is that it forces us to take the child’s perspective. Most things we ask children to do are so obvious to us that it is hard to see what is involved in carrying the tasks. We can learn both about ourselves and well as the children by trying to get at their perspective, by carefully observing HOW the children go about the tasks rather than just noting whether they got the "right" or the "wrong answers."
THE TASKS: (Not in order of presentation to children. That order is presented on the child observation forms).
Give the child 2 opportunities to copy each figure on the observation forms. This task is interesting in two ways. One stems from the fact that the diamond and the square are both squares and yet the diamond is drawn correctly about two years later than the square. How come? What is it about the diagonal orientation that poses special difficulties?
The other is that the tasks give you many opportunities to see HOW the child goes about making the figure. How does she use the standard? Does she know its name, copy it or a prior idea of it, check it out before, during, after? Does the child have a plan? Does he draw straight lines, curved ones, see the connection between lines and corners? Do they improve on their second drawing or get worse? What methods lead to the changes?
Remember, your goal is to see if you can understand their concepts and reasoning. In that regard, it is useful to ask yourself how you "see" the figures so you can copy them? How might you change their reasoning? Why might that fail and what might that tell you about their minds?
PIAGETIAN TASKS. The next three tasks were developed by Piaget and bear on a common theme: multiple-perspective-taking. Imagine a child who seems to understand something from its point of view. What happens if you ask him to look at it from two points of view at the same time, its and yours?
This is a nice straightforward task. It is best when a boy has brothers or a girl has sisters. But it still makes sense when the child has opposite sex siblings. If the child has no siblings, then it loses its point thought you could still ask it out of curiosity--will they make up answers to please you or for some other reason. Or you could explore some related concepts, e.g., parent-grandparent: does your father have a father; is your father a son? Notice the primary point. If "Johnny" has two brothers, Tom and Bill, what will he say when you ask him how many brothers does Billy have? You would say 2 because you would include yourself if you were Johnny. What if the child says 1, Tom? How can you explain such an answer? Plainly the child does not include himself as a brother. Why not? Did he forget and what might such forgetting have to do with the kind of concept of brother he has, with how the terms is used by the child and his family? Are there any things you could do after the testing that would help you understand his answer better? Try explaining to the child the right answer. What happens? What would it mean?
This task has the same idea built into it but doesn't depend on the child having siblings. What is needed by the child is knowledge of the social concention of hand names--my right hand, left hand. Most of the children should know this. If they don't, then the task is inappropriate. But it would be interesting to ask if they'd like you to teach them their hand names and, if they want to, see what happens. But if they do know their hand names, what happens when you ask they to name YOUR hands when they face you? Do they use names corresponding to their point of view of yours? What if they label your left hand as right? Plainly, the child is labeling your hand in terms of how it matches up with its body sides. Are there things you might try that would help you understand his reasoning better? Try explaining the right answer. What happens? What is it about your method that might be confusing, get obedience without understanding, might lead to understanding? In that regard, it is useful to think about your own methods and where they came from.
This is one of Piaget's more famous tasks. It provides an opportunity to discover that some children who can count very well might not know as much about numbers as you might think. Further, it provides a chance to see how many different concepts are embedded in the idea of number and to get an idea of how difficult it is for children to organize them coherently. After determining how the child can count NUMBER NAMES, you present her with two types of counting and conservation problems. In the first, you go through 3 steps. In step 1, you see whether the child can apply the number names to a set of 11 objects. In the second, you see whether she can make comparative number judgements when one row has 6 things and the other has 5 and the two rows are lined up 1-1. (the objects should be as identical as possible in color, shape, etc.; many parents and schools don't like you to use candy. Alternatives might be lottos, coins, raisins.) Step 3 is the CONSERVATION TASK. Which row will the child choose to keep after you spread the row of 5 so that it is longer than the row of 6? Will the child base its choice on number or spread or perhaps choose the row of 6 because it's more "bunched together"? If she chooses the spread row, will she change her mind when asked to count and/or show confusion over what to do? The second task repeats the first, only now a "mistake" would be less dramatic because choice of the spread-out-row still leads the child to get as many things as the choice of the other row. Doing the second task gives you a look at how stable the child's concepts are. For example, if they chose the row of 5 instead of 6, then corrected themselves when they counted, will they now make the mistake again? Or will they now know spreading has nothing to do with "how many"? Again, if they err, how might their minds be working? What happens when you try to explain the right answer? What is it about the explanation that does and doesn't work?
SOME GUIDELINES RELATIVE TO GRADING
C-level work:
Very general descriptions of the two children
Observations that evidence that you understood the point of the tasks and administered and recorded them carefully in accordance with the instructions for the assignment.
Discussion makes reasonable point(s) but they tend to be from the "adult point of view" and tend to be a general descriptive level or the point is not clearly presented or the evidence is not clearly marshalled.
Good English and editing
A-level work:
Particularized descriptions the children that communicate a sense of what they were like as they interacted with you.
Observations that go beyond the descriptive, including "how's" and task "innovations" that clearly bear on providing a picture of the children's concepts and reasoning.
Discussion provides reasonable specific points about how the workings of some of the children's minds on some of the tasks led them to act as they did and provided relevant detailed evidence for the points in a clear, coherent fashion.
A thoughtful commentary on what you learned from the project that takes into account the prior points.
B-level work: Somewhere in between C and A.
REPORT ON THE TRANSCENDENT CHILD
Two parts:Part 1. A brief description of what Rubin means by "transcendence" and a succinct description of her argument about what leads some children to be able transcend their "poisonous" life circumstances.
Part 2. Choose one of the cases that most clearly bears upon someone you know well. In the framework of describing the similarity of the two people,discuss whether Rubin's analysis helps you understand the person you know and discuss questions her perspective raises for you.
My intent is to get you thinking about the relation of childhood to adulthood in a personally meaningful way and not to judge your efforts. Therefore if you take the task seriously and write up your thoughts in good, well-edited English, you are assured a grade of 100 on this assignment. There'll be no comments from me, just a number which I'm sure will be 100 because you will hold up your end of the bargain. The report should be about 5 double-spaced pages in length. It certainly can be longer but try hard not to be wordy or wandering.
GO TO TOP OF PAGE