The
paper below was written 13 years ago and there have been a few developments in
the meantime.
I
am still a professor in the department of psychology at the University of
Houston, although I retired several years ago. After 43 years in Houston,
Carmen and I decided to move to Taos, New Mexico, where we live in an old adobe
house with an acequia and a great view. The University hired me back on a
half-time basis, let me keep my office, and asked me to teachfromTaos via
Distance Learning. I was willing, even
eager, and have not been sorry.
Some
things have changed. I have less to do with NAMI and more to do with the World
Fellowship for Schizophrenia and Allied Disorders, of which I am
president-elect. For one who enjoys travel, it has been good. I have been to
Hamburg, Paris, Jerusalem, Washington, Osaka and Tokyo in recent months. I
completed a term on the National Advisory Mental Health Council and the
American Psychological Association's Task Force on Serious Mental Illness.
Subsequent
to writing the paper below, I became involved in a new area within
developmental psychology. Working with
pediatricians at the University of Texas at the Medical Branch in Galveston I
have been investigating the long-term consequences of otitis media with
effusion. We began with 698 infants and
are now analyzing data at child age seven.
This foray into pediatric psychology has been enormously rewarding.
How
do I spend my retirement years? I do not play golf or tennis, have never had a
talent for fishing and have, wisely, I think, given up rock climbing. I do ski
and hike and enjoy the Taos Archeological Society. Mostly, I read student
papers, and write. I have published 6 books and have 6 more in process on my
computer.
**********
Printed in: Johnson, D. L. (1990). Developing family environments with
families. In F. Kaslow (Ed.). Voices in family psychology. Beverly
Hills, CA: Sage.
"In order to help a
person reach a
certain point, one must
know more
than he knows. But first one must
know what he knows. One must find
him where he is and
begin from there."
S. Kierkegaard
Developing
Family Environments With Families
Dale
L. Johnson
Family Influences
I was born in a house my father built, at least
partly, in Sanish, North Dakota, across the Missouri river from an Indian
reservation, at the end of the Soo Line Railroad, and one of the last of the
true frontier towns in America. It now
lies under Lake Sakakawea.
The priority of family was impressed on me early as
my parents, Kenneth Johnson and Mildred Christensen, sister Pat, and I lived
surrounded by grandparents, aunts, uncles and 28 cousins.
As I small boy I spent my days riding my horse in the
hills, trapping along the Missouri, skiing, skating, and taking full advantage
of the almost limitless freedom granted by my parents. Although Sanish was
devastated by the depression with a drought and by grasshoppers and we had
little money, with a mother who was a gifted teacher (she retired last year at
age 86), a father whose humor and sociability made every day special, and a
house full of teachers and relatives who lived with us for various lengths of
time, life for a small boy was exciting.
I had a remarkable degree of autonomy. Even as a very young child I was free to
come and go as I wished. Only the
Missouri river itself was out of bounds. I was rarely punished. Good behavior
was expected and minor slips were tolerated. It was a rough place to live and
many of the local adolescents were involved in delinguent behavior. If we had
stayed on there I might have joined them.
A sense of social responsibility was extended to the
larger society through the actions of grandfather, Lars Christensen, an immigrant
from Denmark, a homesteader, and one of the founders of the nonpartisan league,
the populist movement that was depicted in the movie, Northern Lights.
Discussion among the relatives was often political and the theme was of the
interplay between social and individual responsibilities. My mother and her
family emphasized the former, my father and his family, the latter. In either case, it was clear that if you
were unhappy with the state of affairs, you should set out to change it and not
expect change to take place because others will rush in to help out. Much later, a year in Norway (1973-1974),
reminded me that this ethic was solidly rooted in the Scandinavian world view
and had been transplanted with little change to the Dakotas.
My childhood years were as nearly ideal as I can
image. In particular, the family environment was secure, loving, intellectually
stimulating, and much of the time uproariously funny.
Educational Influences
My early elementary years were spent in the Sanish
school with its rich mixture of various Anglo and American Indian children and
an exciting meld of cultures. When I was 10 my family moved to another small
town in North Dakota, away from my beloved hills, but to a better job for my
father, managing and eventually owning a hardware and farm implement store.
The high school experience was a page out of Barker's
Big School, Small School (1964).
My school, the Benson County Agricultural Training School, had 125
students in 4 grades, and my senior class had only 24 students. There were so
few students and so many things to be done that every one had to play many
roles. Everyone was responsible, no one
was left out, we were all obligated to one another. Academically, it was not a strong school, but there was a tough
and creative English teacher who made us write and a teacher of agriculture who
introduced us to statistical design and drilled us in practice sessions with
Robert's Rules of Order. He said the
world runs through meetings and we should be prepared to make these meetings as
effective as possible.
Education
From high school I went to the University of North
Dakota in 1947. Although UND provided a
rich experience, I was an inconsistent scholar. My initial major was premedicine, because, among other reasons, I
had been fascinated by the life of the medical researcher in Sinclair
Lewis' Arrowsmith (1925), but I
had little interest in the premedical curriculum. Journalism was of interest as I was doing well financially as a
part-time photographer and the life style seemed exciting. But in my junior year I discovered
psychology and anthropology and knew that I had found what I wanted. Although accepted for medical school, I
chose psychology, and have had no regrets. UND also provided a rich social
experience (it was something of a party school), gave me the chance to gratify
another passion, basketball, and advanced my political development from a
conservative republican to a liberal democrat.
Most important of all, the UND experience brought me to Carmen Acosta,
my wife of 36 years [now 50]. There was
little accident in that meeting: I discovered her, was fascinated by her even
before we had met, asked a cousin to introduce us, and I set out to woo her. The success of that project was the most
important in my life.
I chose Kansas for graduate work in 1951 because
according to the catalog it appeared to offer the psychoanalytic orientation I
thought I wanted. It also offered the opportunity to be trained at the
Menninger Institute. On arrival I discovered that Roger Barker had come in as
chairman and had swept the place clean of psychoanalytic influence. He had repopulated the faculty with
Lewinians and gestalters: Fritz Heider, Martin Scheerer, Erik Wright, Herb
Wright, Al Baldwin, Lee Meyerson, Tony Smith, Bert Kaplan, Anni Frankl, and Ed
Wike. We were immersed in the work of Wertheimer (1945), Koffka (1935), Kohler
(1947), Werner (1948), Lewin (1951), Piaget (1953, 1954) and Goldstein
(1939). Having come from a behaviorist
school and seeking psychoanalytic insights, I was stunned to hear Scheerer in
his first lecture tell us that the major sources of error in American
psychology were behaviorism and psychoanalysis.
The faculty were in no sense dogmatic and loved
academic argument. They believed that students needed to have an understanding
of all major points of view and so brought in as visiting professors Austin
DesLauriers (1962), Paul Bergman, Joseph Nuttin (1953), and Sigmund Gundle (
all psychoanalytic), and Jack Michael (behaviorist).
Today there are few gestalters or Lewinians, but on
the other hand, much of contemporary psychology is based on ideas that have sprung from those
sources. Information processing in
cognitive psychology, ecological psychology, the idea of understanding
environmental forces, holistic approaches, all began there.
My work with Meyerson (Barker, Meyerson, Wright &
Gonick,1953) profoundly affected my understanding of the impact of physical
illness upon behavior and he, more than anyone else, taught me how to think
like a psychologist.
Then John Chotlos entered the scene and I learned to
be skeptical of psychological theories and methods. Chotlos introduced a loyal group of students to the intellectual
pleasures of existential/phenomenological philosophy and psychology. He led a
seminar entitled "Some problems in clinical psychology" which
continued for four years. This seminar
emphasized the importance of understanding direct experience. I did my
dissertation with Chotlos on the moral judgment of schizophrenics.
I think Scheerer was right about psychoanalysis.
Although in contact with psychoanalytic thinking over the years, and briefly in
analytic treatment (four sessions for migraine headaches which were treated
successfully later with ergotamine tartrate), I now find it to be of no value
as a psychotherapy, as a theory of development, or as a guide to understanding
the behavior of individuals or groups.
I think it has retarded the progress of science and contributed a
mythology of human behavior that has been injurious to the seriously mentally
ill, to women, and to families. The Golsteinian (1939) organismic alternative
offered by Scheerer (1954), along with Chotlos' phenomenology, Barkerian
ecology, and the Piagetian view of development have seemed to me much more satisfactory
and scientifically sound sources of theoretical reference.
Opportunities and Other
Influences
Cross cultural
My boyhood interest in cultural issues was reinforced
by marrying Carmen, who had studied in Mexico and had strong interests in the
research area. In graduate school Lee
Meyerson provided us with an opportunity to spend a summer studying the social
situation of the blind among the Mazahua of central Mexico.
We spent four summers working with Bert Kaplan in his
study of the use of native categories for classifying mental disorder with the
Navajo (Kaplan & Johnson, 1964). Later, Carmen and I did similar research
with the Sioux in the Dakotas (Johnson & Johnson, 1965). We also helped the Olympic Mental Center
plan their delivery of services for Indians of western Washington. We recommended that the Center take
advantage of indigenous resources and develop a flexible out-reach system
involving community visiting psychiatric nurses.
There was other field research in Mexico, Guatemala,
and Norway (Johnson, Teigen & Davila, 1983) and in all of this we brought
the children, and often a grandparent, along. There were the hazards of illness
(One child developed malaria in Guatemala. It has not recurred), but we deemed
the advantages of sharing interesting experiences to be greater than the
possible risks.
The cross-cultural work taught me that Pike's idea of
emic (the view of the culture itself) is as important as the etic (the
generalist, universalist view) approach to understanding cultures. Although of
course impressed with cultural differences, I have also been struck by the
great within-culture differences that exist and with the basic similarities of
people everywhere. I cannot accept
explanations of any human behavior as simply "cultural" and prefer to
understand the individuals within the cultural context.
Small Group Research
After graduate school I joined the Veterans
Administration Hospital in Houston in 1957 and worked with seriously mentally ill patients on an acute
admitting ward. In 1960 I became the
director of the new Human Interaction Training Laboratory (HITL) for
psychiatric patients. The HITL was designed by Bob Morton and based on training
principles developed by Robert Blake and Jane Mouton for management training
(Rothaus, Morton, Johnson, Cleveland & Lyle, 1963). All patient groups were
self-directed and time limited. Staff
provided social skills training and participants recorded their impressions of
their group interactions and made use of the feedback this provided. Relations between staff and participants
were highly democratic and much was made of taking responsibility for one's
actions. Groups were also held
responsible for the actions of their members. Men in the program had a variety
of psychiatric problems, chiefly anxiety, depression, and addictive disorders,
and they were encouraged to place their difficulties in a "problems in
living" context. Ideas of illness or therapy were minimized. Program goals were to reduce symptoms by
enhancing social sensitivity, social skills, an internal locus of control, and
a experimental attitude toward problem-solving (Carlson, Johnson & Hanson,
1981). The research carried out with 2000 men in 200 groups was typical of much
of the group dynamics research on small groups that grew out of a Lewinian
orientation (Lewin, 1951). Our evaluation research found the program to be at
least as effective as conventional group psychotherapy programs and with
considerably less cost (Johnson, Hanson, Rothaus, Morton & Lyle, 1965).
I found the opportunity to join training groups as a
participant and as a trainer essential for my own development and much more
important than personal psychotherapy.
Current Interests
By interest, training and the shaping of a
occupational requirements I am a developmental and clinical psychologist.
Through a combination of choice and chance I have been involved for the past
two decades in two different, but related, ways of working with families. Each has involved research, intervention,
and political advocacy.
As a developmentalist, my interests were initially on
the socialization of the child. This
concern meant that I was as much interested in the environment in which the
child developed as in the child's developmental response to these environmental
circumstances. Like many psychologists
of the 1960's I was also interested in how the environment promoted or deterred
the optimal development of the child, and as a clinical psychologist I felt
that one must intervene to improve the chances for optimal development. It has also seemed obvious to me that
environmental influences do not have a one time effect, nor that early
experience has an unalterable determining influence on the life course of
development. Development is
transactional: One is influenced by the environment and influences that
environment throughout the life-span.
Writing this paper has provoked reflection about my
projects. I use the term projects in two of its meanings; as a program
of organized effort, and in the Sartrean existential sense as a projecting of
oneself into the world and creating a future, a being-in-the-world (Sartre,
1956). I have had to ask to what extent my past experiences have structured or
even determined present work, and to what extent this work has been shaped by choices
made on the basis of expected or hoped for outcomes. There is also, of course,
the role of mere chance.
Poverty Families
From the HITL I went across the bayou in 1964 to the
University of Houston to teach developmental and clinical psychology. Soon, however, I was department chairman
(1966-1972) and I became occupied with administrative duties. These
administrative concerns led me into work with families in a way that I did not
expect. Late in 1968, while searching
for funds for graduate student support, Paul Dokecki and I applied for and
received funds to start a Parent-Child Center (PCC). We were soon (1970) able to convert that project into a
Parent-Child Development Center (PCDC), one of three (PCCs) selected by the
Office of Economic Opportunity, under Mary Robinson's direction, to become
research and development centers.
Robinson believed that early childhood education programs for children
of poverty had not done well because they began too late in the child's life
and did not provide for continuity of effects.
Her proposal was to begin within the first three years of the child's
life and to work with parents as well as children to provide continuity. One would have to change environments in
order to have lasting effects. As the
most important environment for nearly all infants and young children is the
family, however defined, it would be necessary to influence family behavior.
Our charge was to develop such a program with very low-income minority families
and to provide them with a wide range of services. The basic goal was to improve the school performance of the
children, but we set as a general goal that the program should optimize child
competence.
This was a challenge for which my background was well
suited. Although I had virtually no
earlier experience, as a professional, with families, I thought that my
involvement with cultural issues, small groups, and program development and
evaluation could be put to good use.
The Parent-Child Development Center (PCDC), a
parent-child education program designed specifically for low-income Mexican
American families, the result of these concerns, has been almost as effective
as we planned it to be, and more effective than I ever expected. At the end of the program, when children
were three, the program mothers, compared with control mothers, were more
affectionate, used more praise and less criticism, and were more encouraging of
their children's verbalizations. Their homes were more stimulating as
educational environments (Andrews et al, 1982; Johnson, 1975). At follow-ups in
the preschool and elementary years, and in junior high school, the program
children were doing better in school and had fewer behavior problems (Johnson
& Breckenridge, 1982; Johnson & Walker, 1986; Johnson & Walker, 1987). The program was not only effective in
improving the children's school success, it was also virtually unique as a
vehicle for the primary prevention of behavior problems and was cited by the
American Psychological Association on prevention as "exemplary"
(Johnson, 1988).
Two years ago, I became the director of an extensive
evaluation of a project similar to the PCDC.
Avance, is a two-year parent education program in San Antonio, directed
by Gloria Rodriquez, which also works with Mexican American families. As in the PCDC much emphasis is put on
parent education, but it differs in that the second year is directed at helping
mothers earn their high school equivalency credentials and in improving their
vocational skills so they can move into better jobs.
Families of the Seriously Mentally Ill
My second introduction to the area of families was
prompted by necessity. The oldest of our three children, a son, developed
schizophrenia in 1972 when he was 19 years of age. Although he was a remarkably
able child, we had become greatly concerned about his use of marijuana, LSD,
and possibly PCP in high school, but he said he had quit drugs. He completed
school near the top of his class, was elected vice president of a large student
body, and went on to Harvard in 1971. My wife and I discovered that adequate
treatment facilities were not available, with or without insurance, and as
parents of a person with schizophrenia we were isolated by our son's mental
health helpers. None of our son's various helpers ever invited us for an
interview, we had to initiate conferences, and one psychiatrist accused us of
having caused our son's mental illness.
This experience of living with schizophrenia has
thrown me back into work with the seriously mentally ill and their families.
Our family's experience with the mental health system, whether the system of
private or public psychiatry, was shocking, appalling, and endlessly
frustrating. Individual practitioners
of all types tended to be caring, if lacking in skills, but the system was not
designed for serious mental illness. We
feared that our son was one more mentally ill person who would simply
"fall through the cracks" and that he would spend his life in an
institution, among the homeless, or perhaps at best isolated in a back room of
our home. Not inclined to be passive, in 1980 we found other families who felt
as we did and organized a group called Citizens for Human Development. We set
out to change the system in Houston and Texas.
Two years later we discovered that an organization called the National
Alliance for the Mentally Ill (NAMI) had been formed in 1979 and we
joined. I was elected to the NAMI board
and served as vice- president[President in 1991]. Carmen is president of the Texas Alliance for the Mentally Ill.
We have liked the fact that NAMI is made up of families, the clients
themselves, and friends of the mentally ill.
We also have supported NAMI's going beyond mutual support to education
and advocacy. As an organization, it is as important to the mentally ill and
their families as my grandfather's Nonpartisan League was to his fellow
farmers. Both organizations were formed to change social systems that were not
responsive to those who were involved in them.
Before long I was co-chair of NAMI's Curriculum and
Training committee which is made up largely of mental health professionals who
are also educators and who have some kind of family involvement with serious
mental illness. The committee's purpose
is to influence the way the mentally ill and their families are treated by
professionals. Through writing books and journal articles, presenting grand
rounds making presentations at conventions, consulting with accrediting groups,
and doing workshops, it has had some success. As each state develops its own Curriculum
and Training Committee there should be an even greater impact on how
professionals work with the mentally ill and their families. My own
contribution to this work has been in studying the experience of living with a
mentally ill person on family members (Johnson, 1986; 1987; in press). In my
own work and in reviewing the literature on the experience of living with
mental illness I have found that families are indeed burdened. The experience is associated with such
higher levels of anxiety, depression, and psychosocial dysfunction that these
families must be considered as a population at risk. In addition, they are burdened economically. Families are bothered by positive symptoms
such as hallucination, delusions, and disturbed thinking, but equally troubled
by the negative symptoms, anhedonia and lack of motivation. Researchers have focused almost exclusively
on parents of the mentally ill and other relatives who are also affected by
living with mental illness, siblings, children of the mentally ill, and
spouses, have been virtually neglected. Finally, it is clear that as the
mentally ill person recovers, the experience of burden lightens.
The Curriculum and Training Committee has had some
success in changing the views of professionals toward the mentally ill and
their families. There is, however, much work to be done. Families of the mentally ill are still
heavily burdened and too often abused. It is a task for researchers to find
ways to relieve the burden and prevent abuse. It is also essential that we know
more about the consequences of living
with mental illness for all family members including such understudied groups
as siblings, adult children of the mentally ill, and spouses. There are other
questions for research: What is the role of the family in treating mental
illness? How can professionals best
work with families? Why do families
give up and why don't they give up?
What are the cultural variations in family experience?
Common Features
These two lines of work with families are less
distinct than they may seem. They have
in common that each group has been underserved and each presents serious
service delivery problems. Furthermore,
interventions for each often have been inappropriate and families have been
derogated and depreciated. Finally, successful ways of intervening could not be
developed until certain problems had been solved.
Families of both types have told professionals that
what they want is information, access to resources, instruction on how to help
their family member, training in how to cope with stress, and the respect of
professionals who help them. They have rejected the idea that they are
deficient as family providers (the once popular idea that low income families
are culturally deficient is repugnant), or that their lives are pathological
(families of the mentally ill do not agree that mental illness is a product of
pathological family interactions).
These families have not asked for therapy for their family problems. Their goals are to help their children to do
well in school and take a solid place in society and to overcome the handicaps
of poverty and social discrimination (low income families) or mental illness
and stigma (families of the mentally ill).
My involvement with these two lines of interest undoubtedly
reflects my own values and beliefs and these are more apparent retrospectively
than they were prior to the actions taken.
These include being against interpretation and for description, being
against controlling families and for offering them opportunities, and being
against deciding for families and for working with them to make decisions that
are grounded in their own best interests as they perceive them to be.
As an professor I teach a graduate course on working
with the families of the mentally ill.
After several sessions in reviewing pertinent literature and moving
toward an understanding of the community context of providing services for the
mentally ill we work together with families at St. Joseph Hospital.
Directions for the Field
Interest in the family seems to be increasing. Schorr (1988) has pointed out that the
American family with high rates of marital splits, large numbers of adolescent
pregnancies, disruptive effects of drugs and alcohol, and often living in
abject poverty is in great trouble, and that the consequences for children are
deplorable. Furthermore, she points out
in the title of her book that solutions are Within our reach, that is,
we already know how to alleviate many of these problems.
Applied psychology has turned away from its
preoccupation with the treatment of symptoms arising from early repressed
conflicts and turned toward what is called cognitive-behavioral psychology with
its here-and-now orientation, concern with thoughts, and emphasis on action. Because so much of this new direction is
based soundly on research, and the effectiveness of interventions that have
emerged has been demonstrated, it can be expected to continue. The general line
of theory and application can also be expected to have an effect on research on
family interventions.
Psychologists seem to be increasingly aware of the
need to work with environments, not just individuals. This concern has been
articulated by such developmental psychologists as Bronfenbrenner (1979) and
Garbarino (1982), but clinicians have been slow to grasp the implications of an
ecological approach. Of course, there are exceptions as in the work of
Lusterman (1988). Briefly, in working with families the first step is to
analyze the life circumstances, the situation, or context of individual
families, identify the major problems, describe the resources that are
available, and find the blocks to attaining individual and family goals. Essential in this process is that the
professional pay close attention to what family members want for themselves and
for others in their family. It is also
essential that attention be given to what the family needs to reach their
goals. In this assessment, the
professional should place heavy emphasis on research-based knowledge. Then, the family's stated wants and the
professional's assessment of needs are examined together for coherence and
compatibility. The process of blending
these two sources of information is an interactive, or transactive, one, with
each being modified and amplified by the other. The process requires a true
collaborative interaction between the family members and the professionals in
designing services that are mutually satisfactory. In the past this process has been neglected in favor of the
authority of the professionals, and families have suffered mistreatment and
neglect.
For families of poverty, the PCDC model was developed
on the basis of this interaction of family wants and research-based definitions
of needs, and the result has been encouraging.
Avance is a newer application of this method, and goes an important step
further in taking into account the importance of the parent's development not
just as a parent, but as persons in their own right.
For families of the mentally ill, the question of
needs has been answered, at least in part, by an interesting series of studies
on interventions with families. The work of Leff and Vaughn (1981) in England,
Falloon, Boyd and McGill (1984) in southern California, and Anderson, Reiss,
and Hogarty (1986) in Pittsburgh has demonstrated that the relapse rate for
persons with schizophrenia can be reduced from about 40% with medication and
supportive care in the nine months following hospitalization to about 10% in
the same period of time with family management training, medication, and
supportive care. These are highly
significant effects. These new family programs stand in contrast to the
conventional systemic or strategic family therapy which has been proclaimed the
therapy of choice for schizophrenics and their families for years (Haley,
1987), but for which there is still no evidence of effectiveness (Terkelsen,
1983).
The new family psychoeducational or family management
programs will probably be extended to other clinical areas. In a sense, this
has already happened in working with children with attention deficit disorders
and their families (Barkley, 1981).
Other likely problem areas are affective disorders, borderline
personality disorders, substance abuse, and child abuse. It seems likely that
as the new family methods are evaluated, their effectiveness will encourage
widespread adoption. In time, they may
be regarded as the first choice for interventions with more traditional
psychotherapies, including family therapy, being reserved for special problems.
It seems to me that psychologists cannot accept the
notion that empirical research on family intervention procedures is not
possible. Of course, all problems that
can be posed cannot be dealt with through empirical research procedures.
Phenomenologists know that. But
one can recast problems, remember the original request for help or family
goals, and carry out research on how effective procedures are in reaching these
goals. This has been done by the Parent-Child Development Center researchers
(Andrews et al, 1984) and by the family management program researchers
mentioned above. Unvalidated claims such as those made by Haley (1987) are
unacceptable.
Plans
Completion of a book on the Houston Parent-Child
Development Center is long overdue and has high priority among my plans. Next I want to do a book on psychosocial
rehabilitation for families and professionals. Other writing plans are farther
down the line.
The opportunity to evaluate the Avance program has
made it possible to take advantage of developments in knowledge about family
environments since the PCDC evaluation was designed. In particular, I now think it is imperative that the affective
characteristics of the child's family environment be considered, and this
aspect has been included in our current work.
Undoubtedly, my work will continue to play on the environment/person
interaction. I will also continue to
develop service delivery methods and in doing this work to reframe how one
thinks about families that give us professionals difficulty. My inclination is that we shouldn't think of "resistant
families," but think of ineffective delivery systems or of not being aware
of what families understand that they want and need. The reframing process rests on a foundation of respect for the
family.
Summary
The two major family projects with which I have been
involved for the past twenty years, work with families of poverty through the
Houston Parent-Child Development Center and Avance and families of the
seriously mentally ill, appear rather different, but, as shown above, actually
have much in common. Most of my
research has been with families as makers and managers of environments for the
development of family members.
It is clear to me that one must work with families,
recognizing their special circumstances, and being guided by what they want at
least as much as what we professionals believe they need. The autonomous, self-generative aspects of
the family must have priority and should be fostered. As we know from the research of Bandura (1982), Seligman
(1975) and others, a sense of
self-efficacy is vital for the development of self-esteem and the ability to
function without burdening depression.
My concern has been that families of poverty and of the seriously
mentally ill are already burdened by forces to a great extent beyond their
control. As a researcher and
practitioner I would like to find ways to lighten these burdens and develop
family strengths to cope better with those remaining.
References
Anderson,
C. M., Reiss, D. J., & Hogarty, G. E. (1986). Schizophrenia in the family: A practitioner's guide to
psychoeducation and management. New York: Guilford.
Andrews, S. R.,
Blumenthal, J. B., Johnson, D. L., Kahn, A. J., Ferguson, C. J., Lasater, T.
M., Malone, P. E., & Wallace, D. B. (1982). The skills of mothering: A
study of Parent Child Development Centers. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development. 47,(6, Serial No. 198).
Bandura, A. (1982).
Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37,
122-147.
Barker, R. G., &
Gump, P. V. (1964). Big school, small school: High school size and student
behavior. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.
Barker, R. G., Meyerson,
L., Wright, B. A., & Gonick, M. R. (1953). Adjustment to physical
handicap and illness (2nd ed.). New York: Social Science Research Council
Bulletin Series.
Barkley, R.
A.(1981). Hyperactive children: A
handbook for diagnosis amd treatment. New York: Guilford.
Bronfenbrenner, U.
(1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Carlson, R. M., Johnson,
D. L., & Hanson, P. G. (1981).
Social sensitivity and self-awareness in group psychotherapy. Small Group Behavior, 12, 183-194.
Des Lauriers, A. M.
(1962). The experience of reality in childhood schizophrenia. New York: International Universities Press.
Falloon, I. R. H., Boyd,
J. L., & McGill, C. W. (1984). Family care of schizophrenia: A
problem-solving approach to the treatment of mental illness. New York:
Guilford.
Garbarino, J. (1982). Children
and families in the social environment. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.
Goldstein, K. (1939). The
organism. New York: American Book Company.
Haley, J. (1987).
Schizophrenics deserve family therapy, not dangerous drugs and management. Family Therapy News, December.
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations.
New York: Wiley.
Johnson, D. L. (1975).
The development of a program for parent-child education among Mexican-Americans
in Texas. In B. Z. Friedlander, G. M.
Sterritt, & G. E. Kirk (Eds.). Exceptional Infant. Vol. 3
(pp. 374-398). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Johnson, D. L.
(1984). The needs of the chronically
mentally ill: As seen by the
consumer. In M. Mirabi (Ed.) The
chronically mentally ill: Research and services. New York: SP Medical and Scientific Books.
Johnson, D. L.
(1987). Effective primary prevention of
behavior problems in Mexican American children. In R. Rodriquez & M.T.Coleman (Eds.) Mental health issues
of the Mexican origin population in Texas.
Austin: Hogg Foundation.
Johnson, D. L. (1987).
Professional family collaboration. In
A. Hatfield (Ed.) Families of the mentally ill: Meeting the challenges.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Johnson, D. L. (In
press). The family's experience of living with mental illness. In H. Lefley & D.L.Johnson (Eds.) Mental
health professionals and families of the mentally ill. Washington: American Psychiatric Association
Press.
Johnson, D. L. (1988).
Primary prevention of behavior problems in young children: The Houston
Parent-Child Development Center.In R. H. Price, E. L Cowen, R. P. Lorion, &
J. Ramos-McKay (Eds.) 14 ounces of prevention: A casebook for practitioners.
Washington: American Psychological Association Press.
Johnson, D. L., Hanson,
P. G., Rothaus, P., Morton, R. B., & Lyle, F. A. (1965). Human relations training for psychiatric
patients: A follow-up study. Journal of Social Psychiatry, 11,
188-196.
Johnson, D. L., &
Johnson, C. A. (1965). Totally discouraged: Depressive syndrome of the Dakota
Sioux. Transcultural Psychiatric
Research, 2, 141-143.
Johnson, D. L., &
Breckenridge, J. N. (1982). The Houston Parent-Child Development Center and the
primary prevention of behavior problems in young children. American Journal
of Community Psychology, 10, 305-316.
Johnson, D. L., Teigen,
K., & Davila, R. (1983). Anxiety and social restriction as experienced by
children in Mexico, Norway and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 439-454.
Johnson, D. L., &
Walker, T. (1986, April). A follow-up evaluation of the Houston Parent-Child
Development Center: School
performance. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago.
Johnson, D. L., &
Walker, T. (1987). The primary
prevention of behavior problems in Mexican-American children. American
Journal of Community Psychology, 15, 375-385.
Johnson,
D. L., & Walker, T. 1986, April). A follow-up evaluation of the Houston
Parent-Child Development Center: School performance. Paper presented at the meeting of the
American Educational Research Association, Chicago.
Kaplan, B., &
Johnson, D. L. (1964). The social
meaning of Navaho psychopathology and psychotherapy. In A. Kiev (Ed.) Magic, faith and healing (pp.
203-229). New York: Free Press.
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles
of gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace.
Kohler, W. (1947). Gestalt
psychology. New York: Liverwright.
Leff, J. P., &
Vaughn, C. E. (1981). The role of maintenance therapy and relatives' expressed
emotion in relapse of schizophrenia: A two-year follow-up. British Journal
of Psychiatry, 139, 102-104.
Lewin,
K. (1951). Field theory in social sciences. New York: Harper.
Lewis,
S. (1925). Arrowsmith. New York: Harcourt.
Lusterman,
D.-D. (1988). Family therapy and schools: An ecosystemic approach. Family
therapy today, 3, (7), 1-3.
Nuttin,
J. (1953). Psychoanalysis and personality. New York: Sheed & Ward.
Piaget,
J. (1953). The moral judgment of the child. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Piaget,
J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.
Rothaus,
P., Morton, R. B., Johnson, D. L., Cleveland, S. E., & Lyle, F. A. (1963).
Human relations training for psychiatric patients. Archives of General Psychiatry, 8, 572-581.
Sartre,
J. P. (1956). Being and nothingness.
New York: Philosophical Library.
Scheerer,
M. (1954). Cognitive theory. In G. Lindzey (Ed.) Handbook of social
psychology (pp. 91-143). New York: Addison-Wesley.
Schorr,
L. (1988). Within our reach:
Breaking the cycle of disadvantage. New York: Doubleday.
Seligman,
M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San
Francisco: Freeman.
Terkelsen,
K. G. (1983). Schizophrenia and the family: Adverse effects of family therapy. Family
Process, 22, 191-201.
Werner,
H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York:
International Universities Press.
Wertheimer,
K. (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper.